A new study has concluded that following a threat, squirrels in the wild use the ambient chatter of birds to help determine when the danger has passed.

Squirrel with robinShare on Pinterest
A new study describes an interesting relationship between bird chatter and squirrel vigilance.

Nature is an unfriendly place for small animals.

For this reason, many have evolved to use information in the environment as a warning system.

For example, some animals have learned to listen to the alarm calls of other species for clues.

As well as increasing their chances of survival, recognizing the alarms of other animals helps species reduce the energy they spend on unnecessary vigilance behavior.

Alarm signals have a wealth of studies behind them. What they have found is that one member of a species will produce a specific sound, and nearby animals will run and hide.

Some animals also use “all clear” signals to let other members of the species know that they are safe. However, there has been much less research into this type of signal.

Earlier work focused on more closely related animals. For instance, one study found that some woodpeckers use the non-alarm calls of other species as a signal that all is well.

A recent study in PLOS One, however, looked at two very different animals: The team investigated how non-alarm birdsong influences squirrel behavior.

Squirrels tend to occupy similar environments to many songbirds but do not follow them or interact with them. As the authors write, “they do not have ‘tight’ ecological relationships with them.”

The researchers theorized that because birds only “chatter” as a group when threat levels are low, squirrels might identify this as a cue to reduce levels of vigilance behavior.

Research has also shown that squirrels react to the alarm calls of certain species, including American robins and black-capped chickadees, but no studies have looked at responses to non-alarm bird chatter.

To investigate, the researchers studied Eastern gray squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis) living wild in Ohio. To elicit a fear response, they played a recording of the red-tailed hawk (Buteo jamaicensis), a common threat to squirrels and birds alike.

Thirty seconds after playing the hawk call, the researchers played either a 3-minute recording of songbird chatter or 3 minutes of ambient sounds with no bird calls.

The scientists observed the squirrels’ behavior before the hawk call and for the duration of the 3-minute recording.

In all, the researchers analyzed data from 54 individual squirrels. They moved to a new location after each trial to ensure that they did not test the same squirrel more than once.

As the researchers expected, the sound of a red-tailed hawk sparked vigilance behaviors, such as fleeing, looking up, or freezing.

However, the squirrels that heard bird chatter following the hawk call carried out fewer fear responses and returned to their foraging activity more quickly than those that heard ambient noise after the hawk. The study authors summarize:

Gray squirrels exposed to bird chatter expressed significantly lower and more rapidly declining levels of vigilance behavior than those exposed to ambient noise, suggesting they used information contained in bird chatter as a cue of safety.”

These results are an interesting addition to the current understanding of how different species listen to and act upon the sounds of nonrelated species. The authors write:

“We knew that squirrels eavesdropped on the alarm calls of some bird species, but we were excited to find that they also eavesdrop on non-alarm sounds that indicate the birds feel relatively safe. Perhaps in some circumstances, cues of safety could be as important as cues of danger.”

Because this study was one of the first to examine how two very different species might use information from non-alarm calls, other studies are likely to follow.

The authors also outline certain limitations to their study. As an example, the chatter recordings also included the sounds of birds walking through dry leaves and fluttering wings. It might have been that the squirrels were responding to these other auditory cues, instead of the birds’ chatter.

The authors end their paper with a note on manmade noise pollution. They theorize that as the noise of humanity steadily grows in volume and covers a broader geographical area, we might begin to drown out some quieter bird species’ chatter.

The study authors explain how the “lack of safety signals might cause squirrels and other eavesdroppers to allocate more energy toward vigilance behaviors and less toward foraging, potentially compromising fitness.” Indeed, this may already be occurring.